women_in_philosophy_thumbnail_0C41651C-695A-494A-973C-0978827C2CD7.jpg
mcgary_award_1200x400.png
thumbnail_IMG_5927_1200x400.jpg
previous arrow
next arrow
PlayPause

Department of Philosophy

implicit

 

What is Implicit Bias?

Data

Examples

Counteracting Implicit Bias

Philosophical Implications of Implicit Bias

Further Information

 

What is Implicit Bias?

Implicit bias is a subtle and pervasive form of bias against members of a group merely in virtue of their membership in that group. As opposed to explicit bias, which can consist in overtly discriminatory beliefs, actions, or institutional policies, implicit bias often consists of unconscious tacit attitudes and unintentional actions towards a group which may be in direct conflict with a person’s explicit beliefs and values. Most implicitly biased actions are performed by people who are not even aware that their actions are biased. Even the most well-meaning and conscientious people have implicit biases. Implicit biases have a negative impact on targets of such bias and on departmental and professional (and other social) climates.

Some Data on Implicit Bias           

  • Doctors misdiagnosed female cardiac patients but not male patients when one of the symptoms listed was 'stress'. When 'stress' was removed as a symptom, leaving only purely physical symptoms, there was no gender difference in diagnosis. “The presence of stress, the researchers explained, sparked a ‘meaning shift’ in which women’s physical symptoms were reinterpreted as psychological, while ‘men's symptoms were perceived as organic whether or not stressors were present.’” See stories here and here.
  • A study of over 300 recommendation letters for medical faculty hired by a large U.S. medical school found that letters for female applicants differed systematically from those for males. Letters written for women were shorter, provided “minimal assurance” rather than solid recommendation, raised more doubts, portrayed women as students and teachers while portraying men as researchers and professionals, and more frequently mentioned women’s personal lives. (F. Trix, C. Psenka, (2003) Discourse & Society , 14.)  (See the report.)
  • In a national study, 238 academic psychologists (118 male, 120 female) evaluated a curriculum vitae randomly assigned a male or a female name. Both male and female participants gave the male applicant better evaluations for teaching, research, and service experience and were more likely to hire the male than the female applicant. (Steinpreis, R., Anders, K., and Ritzke, D. “The Impact of Gender on the Review of the Curricula Vitae of Job Applicants and Tenure Candidates: A National Empirical Study”, Sex Roles, 41: 7/8, 509-528.)
  • A study of over 300 recommendation letters for medical faculty hired by a large U.S. medical school found that letters for female applicants differed systematically from those for males. Letters written for women were shorter, provided “minimal assurance” rather than solid recommendation, raised more doubts, portrayed women as students and teachers while portraying men as researchers and professionals, and more frequently mentioned women’s personal lives. (F. Trix, C. Psenka, (2003) Discourse & Society, 14.)
  • Anonymous review is only rarely practiced in ecology and evolution journals. However, one such journal, Behavioural Ecology, recently started to have all articles reviewed anonymously.  It led to a 33% increase in papers accepted by female authors. (Budden, A., Tregenza, T.,  Aarssen, L., Koricheva, J., Leimu, R. and Lortie, C. (2008), “Double-Blind Review Favours Increased Representation of Female Authors”, Trends in Ecology and Evolution 23:1, 4-6.)
  • When symphony orchestras adopted “blind” auditions by using a screen to conceal candidates’ identities, the hiring of women musicians increased.  (C. Goldin, C. Rouse. (2000), American Economic Review, 90.)
  • Research shows that incongruities between perceptions of female gender roles and leadership roles cause evaluators to assume that women will be less competent leaders. When women leaders provide clear evidence of their competence, thus violating traditional gender norms, evaluators perceive them to be less likeable and are less likely to recommend them for hiring or promotion. (A.H. Eagly, S.J. Karau. (2002), Psychological Review, 109; C.L. Ridgeway. (2001), Journal of Social Issues, 57; M.E. Heilman. (1980), Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 26.)
  • Implicit biases are pervasive: they appear as statistically "large" effects that are often shown by majorities of samples of Americans. Over 80% of web respondents show implicit negativity toward the elderly compared to the young; 75-80% of self-identified Whites and Asians show an implicit preference for racial White relative to Black. (From the Project Implicit®)
  • People differ in levels of implicit bias: implicit biases vary from person to person - for example as a function of the person’s group memberships, the dominance of a person’s membership group in society, consciously held attitudes, and the level of bias existing in the immediate environment. This last observation makes clear that implicit attitudes are modified by experience. (From the Project Implicit®)

Some Examples of Implicit Bias

  • A woman or member of an underrepresented group makes a point in a seminar or colloquium, only to be ignored, and then a white male makes the same point a little later, only to be applauded for his insight.  
  • A speaker or instructor fails to notice or call on women or members of other underrepresented groups.
  • A speaker or instructor allows men or whites to talk without raising their hands but not women or members of other underrepresented groups.
  • When writing recommendation letters, an advisor unintentionally uses different language or focuses on different features when writing for a woman from those she or he uses when writing for a man.  For example, instead of focusing on women’s accomplishments and academic caliber as the advisor would for a male advisee, the advisor focuses on how hard the candidate has worked, includes irrelevant references to her personal life, and makes weaker or ambiguous praise.
  • When reading recommendation letters, an evaluator unintentionally attributes joint work to the collaborator, or interprets ambiguous wording in a way that hurts the candidate.

So You Think You Aren't Implicitly Biased?

  • Participants' self-conceptions as being objective increased the likelihood of exhibiting gender bias in a hiring evaluation task. So being reminded of how objective you think you are actually makes you more gender-biased (Uhlmann and Cohen 2007) .
  • People are better able to detect implicit biases in others than in themselves (Ponin, Lin, Ross 2013) . Studies show that the ‘bias blind spot’ and ‘better-than-average’ effect is not attenuated by increased cognitive ability. See a discussion of the phenomenon here.
  • Participants in implicit bias study tended to ‘redefine’ or ‘construct ad hoc’ criteria they thought were relevant to job success to match their biased expectations. In one study, participants evaluated two hypothetical candidates for a job as chief of police. One candidate had extensive “street” experience but little formal education; the other had extensive formal education but little street experience. When the street-smart candidate was male and the book-smart candidate was female, participants said that street smarts were the most important criteria for being an effective police chief, and recommended promoting the man. However, when the street-smart candidate was female and the book-smart candidate was male, participants said book smarts were more important, and, once again, recommended the man. They unwittingly tailored their judgments about the tools necessary to be a successful police chief to match their gut feeling that the man was better suited than the woman for the job. (Uhlmann and Cohen 2005) .
  • A study targeting specifically the effects of age-and-gender bias on evaluations of philosophical essays found that old men are apparently believed to write better essays than young women. The study also found that being in a good mood enhanced gender biases and being in a bad mood reduced them. (Forgas 2011)

Are You Responsible for Your Implicit Bias?

Counteracting Implicit Bias

  • If you want to prevent it:
    1. Be aware of possible implicit biases you may have and the situations in which you tend to exhibit them.
    2. Be aware of possible implicit biases you may have or trigger in writing letters of recommendation.
    3. Consider  strategies a department can adopt for reducing implicit bias and discrimination generally.
    4. Check out Philosophical Spaces, a blog run by philosophers about professional climate issues in philosophy and what to do about climate problems.
    5. Read Mary Rowe on micro-inequities and strategies for dealing with people who ‘don’t get it’.
    1. Take time to read letters and use explicit reasoning and criteria for evaluation.
    1. Be aware of departmental events and situations that may contribute to implicit bias. For example, are all of your events all late at night when people with children cannot attend?
    2. For more information on how to create and sustain a thriving departmental climate, see the NSF ADVANCE website, “Climate and Culture”.
  • If you see it happen:
    1. Be an “Active Bystander”. For tips on how to do so, see MIT’s “Active Bystander Strategies” website.

Implicit Bias and Peer Review

Philosophical Implications of Implicit Bias

Further Information

      1. NSF ADVANCE’s "Letters of Recommendation"
      2. NSF ADVANCE’s "Bias"

[This power point presentation was used as notes for a talk to a graduate and faculty audience and does not purport to have adequate citation or references. It is only with this understanding that the author has kindly allowed Rutgers Philosophy Department to post this material. Please do not cite or reference without the author's permission.]

[This power point presentation was used as notes for a talk to an undergraduate audience and does not purport to have adequate citation or references. It is only with this understanding that the author has kindly allowed Rutgers Philosophy Department to post this material. Please do not cite or reference without the author's permission.]

Questions, comments, or suggestions about this site should be directed to the This email address is being protected from spambots. You need JavaScript enabled to view it..

 

Return to Climate Home

Prospective Students

New & Noteworthy

Upcoming Events

Thu, Oct 3, 2024 3:00 pm-05:00 pm
Faculty Meeting
Thu, Oct 10, 2024 3:00 pm-05:00 pm
Dept Colloquium presents, Prof. Miriam Schoenfield (UT, Austin)
Thu, Oct 24, 2024 3:00 pm-05:00 pm
Ásta (Duke University)
Fri, Nov 1, 2024 10:00 am-06:00 pm
Conference on Quine and Lewis